Loneliness increases the risk of poor sleep, higher blood pressure, cognitive
and immune decline, depression, and ultimately an earlier death. Why? The
traditional explanation is that lonely people lack life’s advisors: people who
encourage healthy behaviours and curb unhealthy ones. If so, we should invest in
pamphlets, adverts and GP advice: ignorance is the true disease, loneliness just
a symptom.
But this can’t be the full story. Introverts with small
networks aren’t at especial health risk, and people with an objectively full
social life can feel lonely and suffer the consequences. A new review argues
that for the 800,000 UK citizens who experience it all or most of the time,
loneliness itself is the disease: it directly alters our perception, our
thoughts, and the very structure and chemistry of our brains. The authors –
loneliness expert John
Cacioppo, his wife Stephanie
Cacioppo, and their colleague John Capitanio –
build their case on psychological and neuroscientific research, together with
animal studies that help show loneliness really is the cause, not just the
consequence, of various mental and physical effects.
The review suggests
lonely people are sensitive to negative social outcomes and accordingly their
responses in social settings are dampened. We know the former from reaction time
tasks involving negative social words (lonely people respond faster), and tasks
involving the detection of concealed pain in faces (lonely people are extra
sensitive when the faces are dislikeable). Functional imaging evidence also
shows lonely people have a suppressed neural response to rewarding social
stimuli, which reduces their excitement about possible social contact; they also
have dampened activity in brain areas involved in predicting what others are
thinking – possibly a defence mechanism based on the idea that it’s better not
to know. All this adds up to what the authors characterise as a social
"self-preservation mode."
Meanwhile, animal models are helping us to
understand the deeper, biological correlates associated with loneliness. For
mice, being raised in isolation depletes key neurosteroids including one
involved in aggression; it reduces brain myelination, which is vital to brain
plasticity and may account for the social withdrawal and inflexibility seen in
isolated animals; and it can influence gene expression linked to anxious
behaviours.
What about changes to our neural tissue? Human research is
suggestive: in one study, people who self-identified as lonelier were more
likely to develop dementia. Here, initial cognitive decline could be causing
loneliness, but animal work gives us some plausible mechanisms for loneliness’
impact: animals kept in isolation have suppressed growth of new neurons in areas
relating to communication and memory, just as very social periods such as
breeding season see a pronounced spike in growth.
Other basic brain
processes are also upset by isolation. Isolated mice show reduced delta-wave
activity during deep sleep; and their inflammatory responses also change,
meaning that in one study, three in five isolated mice died following an induced
stroke, whereas every one of their cage-sharing peers survived the same
process.
The research is clear that loneliness directly impacts health,
so we need to do what we can to help people free themselves from social
marginalisation. I’ve seen one approach during my time serving with time
banking charities, in which people give their own time in return for someone
else’s in a different situation – a process that can build social networks. Also
the issue is acquiring momentum through the Campaign to End Loneliness
and technology solutions such as the RSA’s Social
Mirror project – an app that tells people about local social groups and
activities. Mainstream health is also picking this up under the term “social
prescription” (physicians advise patients of social groups and activities
and “facilitators” help the patients take up the opportunities). But amongst all
the institutional activity, we mustn’t forget our individual duties: sometimes
all that’s needed is to reach out.
_________________________________
Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J., & Cacioppo, J. (2014). Toward a
neurology of loneliness. Psychological
Bulletin, 140 (6), 1464-1504 DOI: 10.1037/a0037618
Post
written by Alex Fradera (@alexfradera) for the BPS Research
Digest.
No comments:
Post a Comment