There's a brilliant study that came out two weeks ago," Baroness Professor Susan
Greenfield said at a
recent event promoting her new book, "... they took away all [the
pre-teens'] digital devices for five days and sent them to summer camp ... and
tested their interpersonal skills, and guess what, even within five days they'd
changed."
Greenfield highlighted this study
in the context of her dire warnings about the harmful psychological effects of
modern screen- and internet-based technologies. She is clearly tapping into a
wider societal anxiety around how much time we now spend online and plugged in.
But the Baroness' critics
argue
that
her pronouncements are vague, sensationalised and evidence-lite. The fact she
mentioned this specific new study provides a rare opportunity to examine what
she considers to be strong evidence backing her claims. Let's take a
look.
The research team
led by Yalda Uhls studied two groups of pupils at a state school in Southern
California. Both had an average age of 11 years and said they usually spent an
average of 4.5 hours a day texting, watching TV and video-gaming. One group of
51 children was sent on a five-day outdoor education camp 70 miles outside of
California. Mobile devices, computers and TVs were banned. The children lived
together in cabins, went on hikes, and worked as a team to build emergency
shelters. The other group of 54 children attended five days of school as
usual.
On Monday at the beginning of the week, both groups completed two
psychological tests. The first required that they identify the emotions
displayed by photographs of actors' faces. The second involved identifying the
emotions displayed by characters in short video clips of social scenes, in which
the sound was switched off. At the end of the week, on Friday, both groups
completed the tests again.
Uhls and her colleagues highlight the fact
that the summer camp group improved more on the face test over the course of the
week, as compared with the school group. The summer camp group also showed
improvement on the video test, whereas the school group showed no such
improvement (the camp scores rose from 26 per cent correct to 31 per cent; the
school group flatlined at 28 per cent). The researchers' conclusion: "This study
provides evidence that, in five days of being limited to in-person interaction
without access to screen-based or media device for communication, preteens
improved on measures of nonverbal emotion understanding, significantly more than
the control group."
Unfortunately there are a number of acute problems
with this study, which make this conclusion insupportable. Above all, the
experiences of the two groups of children varied in so many different ways,
other than the fact that one group was banned from screen technologies, that it
is impossible to know what factors may have led to any group
differences.
It's also notable that the summer camp group performed worse
at the two tests at the start of the week as compared with the school group. For
example, they began with an average of 14 errors on the face task whereas the
school group made an average of just 9. Perhaps the camp kids were distracted
because they were excited or anxious about the week ahead. We don't know because
the researchers didn't measure any other psychological factors such as mood or
motivation. By the end of the week, the two groups registered a similar number
of errors on the face task. In other words, the technology-free summer camp kids
didn't end the week with super interpersonal skills, they'd merely caught up
with their screen-addled school colleagues.
We can also speculate about
why the school kids didn't show improvement on the video task, whereas the
summer campers did. Perhaps, after a long school week, the children at school
were tired out. The campers, by contrast, may well have been on a high after
their week in the wilderness with friends. Technology might have had nothing to
do with it.
Other problems with the study are more generic, but just as
serious. The children were not randomised to the two conditions. There's no
mention that the people administering the emotional tests were blinded to which
children were allocated to which condition, nor to the aims of the study, which
introduces the risk they might have inadvertently influenced the
results.
In fairness, Uhls and her team admit to many of these
shortcomings in their paper, but it doesn't stop them from interpreting their
results in line with their prior beliefs about the likely harmful effects of
digital technologies, which they outline at the start of their paper. They couch
their findings firmly in the wider context of technology fears, and they hope
their paper will be "a call to action for research that thoroughly and
systematically examines the effects of digital media on children's social
development."
Is it easy to understand why Baroness Professor Greenfield
was pleased with this study. I will leave you to judge whether she was right to
label it "brilliant", and whether the results do anything to support her
arguments about the adverse effects of digital technology on developing
minds.
_________________________________
Uhls, Y., Michikyan, M., Morris, J., Garcia, D., Small, G., Zgourou,
E., & Greenfield, P. (2014). Five days at outdoor education camp without
screens improves preteen skills with nonverbal emotion cues Computers in Human Behavior, 39, 387-392 DOI:
10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.036
No comments:
Post a Comment